Number of items: 10.
2015
Pereira, A., de Mendonca, A., Silva, D., Guerreiro, M., Freeman, J. ORCID: https://orcid.org/0009-0009-6457-8171 and Ellis, J.
(2015)
Enhancing prospective memory in mild cognitive impairment: the role of enactment.
Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 37 (8).
pp. 863-877.
ISSN 1380-3395
doi: https://doi.org/10.1080/13803395.2015.1072499
Kean, R. J., Lamport, D. J. ORCID: https://orcid.org/0000-0002-4592-0439, Dodd, G. F., Freeman, J. ORCID: https://orcid.org/0009-0009-6457-8171, Williams, C. ORCID: https://orcid.org/0000-0003-4452-671X, Ellis, J., Butler, L. and Spencer, J. ORCID: https://orcid.org/0000-0003-2931-7274
(2015)
Chronic consumption of flavanone-rich orange juice is associated with cognitive benefits: an 8-wk randomised, double-blind, placebo-controlled trial in healthy older adults.
American Journal of Clinical Nutrition, 101 (3).
pp. 506-514.
ISSN 0002-9165
doi: https://doi.org/10.3945/ajcn.114.088518
2012
Pereira, A., Ellis, J. and Freeman, J. ORCID: https://orcid.org/0009-0009-6457-8171
(2012)
Is prospective memory enhanced by cue-action semantic relatedness and enactment at encoding?
Consciousness and Cognition, 21 (3).
pp. 1257-1266.
ISSN 1053-8100
doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2012.04.012
Pereira, A., Ellis, J. and Freeman, J. ORCID: https://orcid.org/0009-0009-6457-8171
(2012)
The effects of age, enactment, and
cue-action relatedness on memory for
intentions in the Virtual Week task.
Aging, Neuropsychology and Cognition, 19 (5).
pp. 549-565.
ISSN 1744-4128
doi: https://doi.org/10.1080/13825585.2011.638977
2008
Ellis, J.A. and Freeman, J.E. ORCID: https://orcid.org/0009-0009-6457-8171
(2008)
Ten years on: realizing delayed intentions.
In: Kliegel, M., McDaniel, M.A. and Einstein, G.O. (eds.)
Prospective memory: cognitive, neuroscience, developmental, and applied perspectives.
Psychology Press, Hove, pp. 1-28.
ISBN 9780805858587
2007
Eschen, A., Freeman, J. ORCID: https://orcid.org/0009-0009-6457-8171, Dietrich, T., Martin, M., Ellis, J., Martin, E. and Kliegel, M.
(2007)
Motor brain regions are involved in the encoding of delayed intentions: a fMRI study.
International Journal of Psychophysiology, 64 (3).
pp. 259-268.
ISSN 0167-8760
doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2006.09.005
2004
Moulin, C.J.A., James, N., Freeman, J.E. ORCID: https://orcid.org/0009-0009-6457-8171 and Jones, R.W.
(2004)
Deficient acquisition and consolidation: intertrial free recall performance in Alzheimer's disease and mild cognitive impairment.
Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 26 (1).
pp. 1-10.
ISSN 1380-3395
doi: https://doi.org/10.1076/jcen.26.1.1.23940
2003
Freeman, J.E. ORCID: https://orcid.org/0009-0009-6457-8171 and Ellis, J.A.
(2003)
Aging and the accessibility of performed and to-be-performed actions.
Aging Neuropsychology and Cognition, 10 (4).
pp. 298-309.
ISSN 1382-5585
doi: https://doi.org/10.1076/anec.10.4.298.28975
Freeman, J. E. ORCID: https://orcid.org/0009-0009-6457-8171 and Ellis, J. A.
(2003)
The intention-superiority effect for naturally occurring activities: the role of intention accessibility in everyday prospective remembering in young and older adults.
International Journal of Psychology, 38 (4).
pp. 215-228.
ISSN 0020-7594
doi: https://doi.org/10.1080/00207590244000205
Freeman, J. E. ORCID: https://orcid.org/0009-0009-6457-8171 and Ellis, J. A.
(2003)
The representation of delayed intentions: a prospective subject-performed task?
Journal of Experimental Psychology-Learning Memory and Cognition, 29 (5).
pp. 976-992.
ISSN 0278-7393
doi: https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.29.5.976
This list was generated on Fri Nov 8 22:19:15 2024 UTC.