Visual processing deficits in children with slow RAN performanceStainthorp, R. W., Stuart, M., Powell, D. ORCID: https://orcid.org/0000-0002-3607-2407, Quinlan, P. and Garwood, H. (2010) Visual processing deficits in children with slow RAN performance. Scientific Studies of Reading, 14 (3). pp. 266-292. ISSN 1088-8438
It is advisable to refer to the publisher's version if you intend to cite from this work. See Guidance on citing. To link to this item DOI: 10.1080/10888431003724070
DownloadsDownloads per month over past year
• Ackerman, P. T., & Dykman, R. A. (1993). Phonological processes, confrontation naming and immediate memory in dyslexia. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 26, 597-690.
• Bach, M. (1996). The “Freiburg visual acuity test”: automatic measurement of visual acuity. Optometry and Vision Science, 73, 49-53.
• Badian, N. A. (1993). Phonemic awareness, naming, visual symbol processing, and reading. Reading and Writing: an Interdisciplinary Journal, 5, 87-100.
• Badian, N. A. (1994). Preschool prediction: Orthographic and phonological skills and reading. Annals of Dyslexia, 44, 3-25.
• Bender, L. A. (1938). Visual-motor gestalt test and its clinical use. Research monograph no. 3, New York: American Orthopsychiatric Association.
• Blachman, B. A. (1984). Relationship of rapid naming ability and language analysis skills to kindergarten and first-grade reading achievement. Journal of Educational Psychology, 76, 610-622.
• Bosse, M-L., Tainturier, M. J., & Valdois, S. (2007). Developmental dyslexia: The visual attention span deficit hypothesis. Cognition, 104, 198-230.
• Bowers, P. G. (1995). Tracing symbol naming speed's unique contributions to reading disabilities over time. Reading and Writing: an Interdisciplinary Journal, 7, 189-216.
• Bowers, P. G. & Newby-Clark, E. (2002). The Role of Naming Speed within a Model of Reading Acquisition. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 15, 109-26.
• Bowers, P. G., Steffy, R., & Tate, E. (1998). Comparison of the effects of IQ control methods on memory and naming speed predictors of reading disability. Reading Research Quarterly, 23, 304-309.
• Bowers, P. G., & Wolf, M. (1993). Theoretical links among naming speed, precise timing mechanisms and orthographic skill in dyslexia. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 5, 69-85.
• Breznitz, Z. (2002). Asynchrony of visual-orthographic and auditory-phonological word recognition processes: An underlying factor in dyslexia. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 15, 15-42.
• Brunsdon, R., Coltheart, M., & Nickels, L. (2006). Severe developmental letter-processing impairment: a treatment case study. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 23, 795-821.
• Clarke, P., Hulme, C., & Snowling, M. (2005). Individual differences in RAN and reading: a response timing analysis. Journal of Research in Reading, 28, 73-86.
• Cobbold, S., Passenger, T., & Terrell, C. (2003). Serial naming speed and the component elements of speech time and pause time: relationships with the development of word-level reading in children aged four to five years . Journal of Research in Reading, 26, 165-176.
• Denckla, M. B., & Rudel, R. G. (1974). Rapid 'automatized' naming of pictured objects, colors, letters and numbers by normal children. Cortex, 10, 186-202.
• Denckla, M. B., & Rudel, R. G. (1976). Rapid automatized naming (RAN) - Dyslexia differentiated from other learning disabilities. Neuropsychologia, 14, 471-479.
• Elliot, C. D., Murray, D. J., & Pearson, L. S. (1983). British Ability Scales. Windsor UK: NFER-Nelson.
• Felton, R. H., & Brown, I. S. (1990). Phonological processes as predictors of specific reading skills in children at risk for reading failure. Reading and Writing: an Interdisciplinary Journal, 2, 39-59.
• Georgiou, G. K., Parrila, R., & Kirby, J. (2006). Rapid naming speed components and early reading acquisition. Scientific Studies of Reading, 10,199-220.
• Gibson, E. J., Gibson, J. J., Pick, A. D., & Osser, H. (1962). A developmental study of the discrimination of letter-like forms. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 55, 897-906.
• Hari, R., & Revall, H. (2001). Impaired processing of rapid stimulus sequences in dyslexia. Trend in Cognitive Sciences, 5, 525-532.
• Holmes, V., & Ng, E. (1993). Word specific knowledge, word-recognition strategies, and spelling ability. Journal of Memory and Language, 32, 230-257.
• Kail, R. (1991). Development of processing speed in childhood and adolescence. Advances in Child Development and Behavior, 23, 151-185.
• Kail, R., & Hall, L. K. (1994). Processing speed, naming speed, and reading. Developmental Psychology, 30, 949-954.
• Kail, R., Hall, L. K., & Caskey, B. J. (1999). Processing speed, exposure to print, and naming speed. Applied Psycholinguistics, 20, 303-314.
• Lachmann, T., & van Leeuven, C. (2007). Paradoxical enhancement of letter recognition in developmental dyslexia. Developmental Neuropsychology, 3, 61-77.
• Lovegrove, W., Martin, F. & Slaghuis,W. (1986). A theoretical and experimental case for a visual deficit in specific reading disability. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 3, 225-267.
• Manis, F. R., Seidenberg, M. S., & Doi, L. M. (1999). See Dick RAN: Rapid naming and the longitudinal prediction of reading subskills in first and second graders. Scientific Studies of Reading, 3, 129-157.
• McClelland, J. L., & Rumelhart, D. E. (1981). An interactive activation model of context effects in letter perception, 1: An account of basic findings. Psychological Review, 88, 375-407.
• Moll, K., Fussenegger, B., Willburger, E. & Landerl, K. (2009) RAN is not a measure of orthographic processing. Evidence from the asymmetric German orthography. Scientific Studies of Reading, 13, 1-25.
• Neuhaus,G., Foorman, B. R., Francis, D. J. & Carlson, C. D. (2001). Measures of information processing in Rapid Automatized Naming (RAN) and their relation to reading. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 78, 359-373.
• Neuhaus, G. F., & Swank, P. R. (2002). Understanding the relations between RAN letter subtest components and word reading in first-grade students. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 35, 158-74.
• Powell, D., Stainthorp, R., Stuart, M., Garwood, H., & Quinlan, P. (2007). An experimental comparison between rival theories of Rapid Automatized Naming (RAN) performance and its relationship to reading. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 96, 46-68.
• Schneider, W., Eschman, A., & Zuccolotto, A. (2002). E-Prime User's Guide. Pittsburgh: Psychology Software.
• Spring, C., & Capps, C. (1974). Encoding Speed, Rehearsal, and Probed Recall of Dyslexic Boys. Journal of Educational Psychology, 66, 780-786.
• Stein, J., & Walsh, V. (1997). To see but not to read: The magnocellular theory of dyslexia. Trends in Neuroscience, 20, 147-152.
• Torgesen, J. K., Wagner, R. K., Rashotte, C. A., Burgess, S., & Hecht, S. (1997). Contributions of phonological awareness and rapid automatic naming ability to the growth of word-reading skills in second- to fifth-grade children. Scientific Studies of Reading, 1, 161-185.
• Vellutino, F. R. (1979). Dyslexia: theory and research. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press.
• Venezky, R. (1993). History of interest in the visual component of reading. In D. M. Willows, R. S. Kruk & E. Corcos (Eds.), Visual processing in reading and reading disabilities. (pp 3 – 30). Hillsdale NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates
• Wagner, R. K., & Torgesen, J. K. (1987). The nature of phonological processing and its causal role in the acquisition of reading skills. Psychological Bulletin, 101, 192-212.
• Wagner, R. K., Torgesen, J. K., & Rashotte, C. A. (1999). Comprehensive test of phonological processing. Austin, TX: PRO-ED.
• Wagner, R. K., Torgesen, J. K., Rashotte, C. A., Hecht, S. A., Barker, T. A., Burgess, S. R., et al. (1997). Changing relations between phonological processing abilities and word-level reading as children develop from beginning to skilled readers: a 5-year longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology, 33, 468-479.
• Wechsler, D. (1992). Wechsler intelligence scale for children. 3rd. edition. New York: Psychological Corporation.
• Willows, D. M., Kruk, R., & Corcos, E. (1993). Are there differences between disabled and normal readers in their processing of visual information? In D. M. Willows, R. Kruk & E. Corcos (Eds.). Visual Processing in Reading and Reading Disabilities. Hillsdale, NJ.: Lawrence Erlbaum Publishers.
• Wolf, M., & Bowers, P. G. (1999). The double-deficit hypothesis for the developmental dyslexias. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91, 415-438.
• Wolf, M., Bowers, P. G., & Biddle, K. (2000). Naming-speed processes, timing, and reading: A conceptual review. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 33, 387-407.
• Wolf, M., & Obregon, M. (1992). Early naming deficit, developmental dyslexia and a specific deficit hypothesis. Brain and Language, 42, 219-247. University Staff: Request a correction | Centaur Editors: Update this record |