Visual processing deficits in children with slow RAN performance
Stainthorp, R. W., Stuart, M., Powell, D.
It is advisable to refer to the publisher's version if you intend to cite from this work. See Guidance on citing. To link to this item DOI: 10.1080/10888431003724070
DownloadsDownloads per month over past year
• Ackerman, P. T., & Dykman, R. A. (1993). Phonological processes, confrontation naming and immediate memory in dyslexia. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 26, 597-690.
• Bach, M. (1996). The “Freiburg visual acuity test”: automatic measurement of visual acuity. Optometry and Vision Science, 73, 49-53.
• Badian, N. A. (1993). Phonemic awareness, naming, visual symbol processing, and reading. Reading and Writing: an Interdisciplinary Journal, 5, 87-100.
• Badian, N. A. (1994). Preschool prediction: Orthographic and phonological skills and reading. Annals of Dyslexia, 44, 3-25.
• Bender, L. A. (1938). Visual-motor gestalt test and its clinical use. Research monograph no. 3, New York: American Orthopsychiatric Association.
• Blachman, B. A. (1984). Relationship of rapid naming ability and language analysis skills to kindergarten and first-grade reading achievement. Journal of Educational Psychology, 76, 610-622.
• Bosse, M-L., Tainturier, M. J., & Valdois, S. (2007). Developmental dyslexia: The visual attention span deficit hypothesis. Cognition, 104, 198-230.
• Bowers, P. G. (1995). Tracing symbol naming speed's unique contributions to reading disabilities over time. Reading and Writing: an Interdisciplinary Journal, 7, 189-216.
• Bowers, P. G. & Newby-Clark, E. (2002). The Role of Naming Speed within a Model of Reading Acquisition. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 15, 109-26.
• Bowers, P. G., Steffy, R., & Tate, E. (1998). Comparison of the effects of IQ control methods on memory and naming speed predictors of reading disability. Reading Research Quarterly, 23, 304-309.
• Bowers, P. G., & Wolf, M. (1993). Theoretical links among naming speed, precise timing mechanisms and orthographic skill in dyslexia. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 5, 69-85.
• Breznitz, Z. (2002). Asynchrony of visual-orthographic and auditory-phonological word recognition processes: An underlying factor in dyslexia. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 15, 15-42.
• Brunsdon, R., Coltheart, M., & Nickels, L. (2006). Severe developmental letter-processing impairment: a treatment case study. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 23, 795-821.
• Clarke, P., Hulme, C., & Snowling, M. (2005). Individual differences in RAN and reading: a response timing analysis. Journal of Research in Reading, 28, 73-86.
• Cobbold, S., Passenger, T., & Terrell, C. (2003). Serial naming speed and the component elements of speech time and pause time: relationships with the development of word-level reading in children aged four to five years . Journal of Research in Reading, 26, 165-176.
• Denckla, M. B., & Rudel, R. G. (1974). Rapid 'automatized' naming of pictured objects, colors, letters and numbers by normal children. Cortex, 10, 186-202.
• Denckla, M. B., & Rudel, R. G. (1976). Rapid automatized naming (RAN) - Dyslexia differentiated from other learning disabilities. Neuropsychologia, 14, 471-479.
• Elliot, C. D., Murray, D. J., & Pearson, L. S. (1983). British Ability Scales. Windsor UK: NFER-Nelson.
• Felton, R. H., & Brown, I. S. (1990). Phonological processes as predictors of specific reading skills in children at risk for reading failure. Reading and Writing: an Interdisciplinary Journal, 2, 39-59.
• Georgiou, G. K., Parrila, R., & Kirby, J. (2006). Rapid naming speed components and early reading acquisition. Scientific Studies of Reading, 10,199-220.
• Gibson, E. J., Gibson, J. J., Pick, A. D., & Osser, H. (1962). A developmental study of the discrimination of letter-like forms. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 55, 897-906.
• Hari, R., & Revall, H. (2001). Impaired processing of rapid stimulus sequences in dyslexia. Trend in Cognitive Sciences, 5, 525-532.
• Holmes, V., & Ng, E. (1993). Word specific knowledge, word-recognition strategies, and spelling ability. Journal of Memory and Language, 32, 230-257.
• Kail, R. (1991). Development of processing speed in childhood and adolescence. Advances in Child Development and Behavior, 23, 151-185.
• Kail, R., & Hall, L. K. (1994). Processing speed, naming speed, and reading. Developmental Psychology, 30, 949-954.
• Kail, R., Hall, L. K., & Caskey, B. J. (1999). Processing speed, exposure to print, and naming speed. Applied Psycholinguistics, 20, 303-314.
• Lachmann, T., & van Leeuven, C. (2007). Paradoxical enhancement of letter recognition in developmental dyslexia. Developmental Neuropsychology, 3, 61-77.
• Lovegrove, W., Martin, F. & Slaghuis,W. (1986). A theoretical and experimental case for a visual deficit in specific reading disability. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 3, 225-267.
• Manis, F. R., Seidenberg, M. S., & Doi, L. M. (1999). See Dick RAN: Rapid naming and the longitudinal prediction of reading subskills in first and second graders. Scientific Studies of Reading, 3, 129-157.
• McClelland, J. L., & Rumelhart, D. E. (1981). An interactive activation model of context effects in letter perception, 1: An account of basic findings. Psychological Review, 88, 375-407.
• Moll, K., Fussenegger, B., Willburger, E. & Landerl, K. (2009) RAN is not a measure of orthographic processing. Evidence from the asymmetric German orthography. Scientific Studies of Reading, 13, 1-25.
• Neuhaus,G., Foorman, B. R., Francis, D. J. & Carlson, C. D. (2001). Measures of information processing in Rapid Automatized Naming (RAN) and their relation to reading. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 78, 359-373.
• Neuhaus, G. F., & Swank, P. R. (2002). Understanding the relations between RAN letter subtest components and word reading in first-grade students. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 35, 158-74.
• Powell, D., Stainthorp, R., Stuart, M., Garwood, H., & Quinlan, P. (2007). An experimental comparison between rival theories of Rapid Automatized Naming (RAN) performance and its relationship to reading. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 96, 46-68.
• Schneider, W., Eschman, A., & Zuccolotto, A. (2002). E-Prime User's Guide. Pittsburgh: Psychology Software.
• Spring, C., & Capps, C. (1974). Encoding Speed, Rehearsal, and Probed Recall of Dyslexic Boys. Journal of Educational Psychology, 66, 780-786.
• Stein, J., & Walsh, V. (1997). To see but not to read: The magnocellular theory of dyslexia. Trends in Neuroscience, 20, 147-152.
• Torgesen, J. K., Wagner, R. K., Rashotte, C. A., Burgess, S., & Hecht, S. (1997). Contributions of phonological awareness and rapid automatic naming ability to the growth of word-reading skills in second- to fifth-grade children. Scientific Studies of Reading, 1, 161-185.
• Vellutino, F. R. (1979). Dyslexia: theory and research. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press.
• Venezky, R. (1993). History of interest in the visual component of reading. In D. M. Willows, R. S. Kruk & E. Corcos (Eds.), Visual processing in reading and reading disabilities. (pp 3 – 30). Hillsdale NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates
• Wagner, R. K., & Torgesen, J. K. (1987). The nature of phonological processing and its causal role in the acquisition of reading skills. Psychological Bulletin, 101, 192-212.
• Wagner, R. K., Torgesen, J. K., & Rashotte, C. A. (1999). Comprehensive test of phonological processing. Austin, TX: PRO-ED.
• Wagner, R. K., Torgesen, J. K., Rashotte, C. A., Hecht, S. A., Barker, T. A., Burgess, S. R., et al. (1997). Changing relations between phonological processing abilities and word-level reading as children develop from beginning to skilled readers: a 5-year longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology, 33, 468-479.
• Wechsler, D. (1992). Wechsler intelligence scale for children. 3rd. edition. New York: Psychological Corporation.
• Willows, D. M., Kruk, R., & Corcos, E. (1993). Are there differences between disabled and normal readers in their processing of visual information? In D. M. Willows, R. Kruk & E. Corcos (Eds.). Visual Processing in Reading and Reading Disabilities. Hillsdale, NJ.: Lawrence Erlbaum Publishers.
• Wolf, M., & Bowers, P. G. (1999). The double-deficit hypothesis for the developmental dyslexias. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91, 415-438.
• Wolf, M., Bowers, P. G., & Biddle, K. (2000). Naming-speed processes, timing, and reading: A conceptual review. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 33, 387-407.
• Wolf, M., & Obregon, M. (1992). Early naming deficit, developmental dyslexia and a specific deficit hypothesis. Brain and Language, 42, 219-247. University Staff: Request a correction | Centaur Editors: Update this record |