Bach, M., & Meigen, T. (1997). Similar electrophysiological correlates of texture segregation induced by luminance, orientation, motion and stereo. Vision Research, 37(11), 1409–1414. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0042-6989(96)00322-7
Beck, J. (1966). Effect of orientation and of shape similarity on perceptual grouping. Perception & Psychophysics, 1(9), 300–302. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03215792
Ben-Shahar, O., & Zucker, S. W. (2004). Sensitivity to curvatures in orientation-based texture segmentation. Vision Research, 44(3), 257–277. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. visres.2003.08.018
Caputo, G., & Casco, C. (1999). A visual evoked potential correlate of global figure- ground segmentation. Vision Research, 39(9), 1597–1610. https://doi.org/10.1016/ S0042-6989(98)00270-3
Cerkevich, C. M., Lyon, D. C., Balaram, P., & Kaas, J. H. (2014). Distribution of cortical neurons projecting to the superior colliculus in macaque monkeys. Eye and brain, 6 (Suppl 1), 121.
Coren, S., & Hoenig, P. (1972). Effect of non-target stimuli upon length of voluntary saccades. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 34(2), 499–508. https://doi.org/10.2466/ pms.1972.34.2.499
Cornsweet, T. (1970). Visual Perception. New York: Academic Press.
Deubel, H., Findlay, J., Jacobs, A., & Brogan, D. (1988). Saccadic eye movements to
targets defined by structure differences. In G. Lüer, U. Lass, & J. Schallo-Hoffmann (Eds.), Eye movement research: Physiological and psychological aspects (pp. 107–145). Toronto: Hogrefe.
Deubel, H., & Frank, H. (1991). The latency of saccadic eye movements to texture- defined stimuli. In R. Schmid, & D. Zambarbieri (Eds.), Oculomotor control and cognitive processes (pp. 369–384). North-Holland: Elsevier.
Findlay, J. M. (1982). Global visual processing for saccadic eye movements. Vision Research, 22(8), 1033–1045. https://doi.org/10.1016/0042-6989(82)90040-2
He, P., & Kowler, E. (1992). The role of saccades in the perception of texture patterns. Vision Research, 32(11), 2151–2163. https://doi.org/10.1016/0042-6989(92)90076- U
Itti, L., & Koch, C. (2000). A saliency-based search mechanism for overt and covert hifts of visual attention. Vision Research, 40(10–12), 1489–1506. https://doi.org/ 10.1016/S0042-6989(99)00163-7
Julesz, B. (1981). Textons, the elements of texture perception, and their interactions. Nature, 290, 91–97.
Kingdom, F. A. A., & Keeble, D. R. T. (1996). A linear systems approach to the detection of both abrupt and smooth spatial variations in orientation-defined textures. Vision Research, 36(3), 409–420. https://doi.org/10.1016/0042-6989(95)00123-9
Lamme, V. A. F. (1995). The neurophysiology of figure-ground segregation in primary visual cortex. The Journal of Neuroscience, 15(2), 1605–1615.
Lamme, V. A. F., Rodriguez-Rodriguez, V., & Spekreijse, H. (1999). Separate Processing Dynamics for Texture Elements, Boundaries and Surfaces in Primary Visual Cortex of the Macaque Monkey. Cerebral Cortex, 9(4), 406–413. https://doi.org/10.1093/ cercor/9.4.406
Lamme, V. A. F., & Roelfsema, P. R. (2000). The distinct modes of vision offered by feedforward and recurrent processing. Trends in Neurosciences, 23(11), 571–579. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0166-2236(00)01657-X
Vision Research 210 (2023) 108264
Lamme, V. A. F., Zipser, K., & Spekreijse, H. (1998). Figure-ground activity in primary visual cortex is supressed by anesthesia. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 95(6), 3263–3268. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.95.6.3263
Landy, M. S., & Bergen, J. R. (1991). Texture segregation and orientation gradient. Vision Research, 31(4), 679–691. https://doi.org/10.1016/0042-6989(91)90009-T
Melcher, D., & Kowler, E. (1999). Shapes, surfaces and saccades. Vision Research, 39(7), 2929–2946. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0042-6989(99)00029-2
Norman, L. J., Heywood, C. A., & Kentridge, R. W. (2011). Contrasting the processes of texture segmentation and discrimination with static and phase-reversing stimuli. Vision Research, 51, 2039–2047. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.visres.2011.07.021
Nothdurft, H. C. (1985). Orientation sensitivity and texture segmentation in patterns with different line orientation. Vision Research, 25(4), 551–560. https://doi.org/ 10.1016/0042-6989(85)90159-2
Nothdurft, H. C. (1993). The role of features in preattentive vision: Comparison of orientation, motion and color cues. Vision Research, 33(14), 1937–1958. https://doi. org/10.1016/0042-6989(93)90020-W
Nothdurft, H. C., & Parlitz, D. (1993). Absence of express saccades to texture or motion defined targets. Vision Research, 33(10), 1367–1383. https://doi.org/10.1016/0042- 6989(93)90044-W
Olsen, A. (2012). The Tobii I-VT fixation filter. Tobii Technology, 21, 4–19. Parkhurst, D., Law, K., & Niebur, E. (2002). Modeling the role of salience in the
allocation of overt visual attention. Vision Research, 42(1), 107–123. https://doi.org/
10.1016/S0042-6989(01)00250-4
Pierce, J. (2007). PsychoPy—Psychophysics software in Python. Journal of Neuroscience
Methods, 162(1–2), 8–13.
Ploner, C. J., Ostendorf, F., & Dick, S. (2004). Target Size Modulates Saccadic Eye
Movements in Humans. Behavioral Neuroscience. https://doi.org/10.1037/0735-
7044.118.1.237
Poort, J., Raudies, F., Wannig, A., Lamme, V. A. F., Neumann, H., & Roelfsema, P. R. (2012). The role of attention in figure-ground segregation in areas V1 and V4 of the visual cortex. Neuron, 75, 143–156. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2012.04.032
Rossi, A. F., Desimone, R., & Ungerleider, L. G. (2001). Contextual Modulation in Primary Visual Cortex of Macaques. The Journal of Neuroscience, 21(5), 1698–1709.
Scholte, H. S., Jolij, J., Fahrenfort, J. J., & Lamme, V. A. F. (2008). Feedforward and Recurrent Processing in Scene Segmentation: Electroencephalography and Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 20(11), 2097–2109.
Sup`er, H., & Lamme, V. A. F. (2007). Strength of figure-ground activity in monkey primary visual cortex predicts saccadic reaction time in a delayed detection task. Cerebral Cortex, 17(6), 1468–1475. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhl058
Sup`er, H., Spekreijse, H., & Lamme, V. A. F. (2001). Two distinct modes of sensory processing observed in monkey primary visual cortex (V1). Nature Neuroscience, 4 (3), 304–310. https://doi.org/10.1038/85170
Sup`er, H., Spekreijse, H., & Lamme, V. A. F. (2003). Figure-ground activity in primary visual cortex (V1) of the monkey matches the speed of behavioral response. Neuroscience Letters, 344(2), 75–78. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0304-3940(03) 00360-4
Wolfson, S. S., & Landy, M. S. (1998). Examining edge- and region-based texture analysis mechanisms. Vision Research, 38(3), 439–446. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0042-6989 (97)00153-3